What We Understand about Cats and What They Understand about Us
A
big part of cats' lives are spent around their human owners, yet
scientists are just starting to understand what they think of us
By Felicity Muth on March 30, 2016
In my last post
I introduced the topic of cat cognition and what we broadly know about
how these animals think. In this post I'm going to talk more
specifically about what we understand about cats' interactions with the
animal they spend most time with: us.
Sensitivity to human cues
Since cats have both been bred to be domestic and spend a lot of time
with humans, we would expect them to pick up on human cues to some
extent. However, anyone who has owned a cat knows that they are not
always as responsive as you might want them to be.
One way in which we frequently attempt to interact with the animals
that live with us is by pointing at things. It is possible that this
shows our limitations rather than our animal friends since this is a
particularly human means of communication. However, in 2005 a study by
Miklósi et al. demonstrated that cats could indeed follow human gestures
to find food. The researchers also investigated whether, when unable to
solve a task, whether the cats turned to the humans for help at all.
They did not. Cats seem to be able to understand pointing
Another study looked to see whether cats turn to humans when unsure
about a certain situation. This ‘social referencing’ is something that
we do both as children and as adults, for example a clown might
initially seem terrifying but if everyone else is having a good time we
may quickly learn that this isn’t a situation to be feared (there are
always exceptions to this of course). To see whether cats do this too,
the researchers exposed cats to a potentially scary fan with streamers.
The cat was brought into a room with their owner and the fan was put on.
The owner was then told to act either neutral, scared of the fan, or
happy and relaxed around the fan. The researchers found that most cats
(79%) looked between the fan and their human owner, seeming to gage
their response. The cats also responded to the emotional response of
their owner, being more likely to move away from the fan when their
owner was looking scared, as well as being more likely to interact with
their owner. It’s difficult to know how to interpret this, but the
authors suggest that the cats may have been seeking security from their
owner.
Other research has also shown that cats are sensitive to human moods,
being less likely to approach people who were feeling sad and more
likely to approach people who described themselves as feeling
extroverted or agitated. However, why this should be isn’t clear. Believe it or not, cats respond to their owner's moodHuman voice recognition
Two researchers, Saito and Shinozuka in 2013 demonstrated that cats
can recognise their owner’s voice. To test this, the researchers played
cats recordings of either their owner calling them or other people
calling their name. The cats were the most responsive to their owner
calling. This response was mostly seen in terms of the cat moving its
ears or head, rather than walking towards the voice as a dog might.
Vocal communication
Kittens have around 9 different types of vocalisation, while adults
have around 16 different types. Interestingly, domestic and feral cats
also differ from each other in their vocalisations, implying that their
relationships with humans influences how cats ‘talk’. Perhaps one of the
most renowned vocalisations of cats is their purr. Cats don’t just purr
when being stroked by humans, they also use it in interactions with
each other and with their kittens. What’s more, cats alter their purr to
change the meaning of the vocalization. For example, when asking for
food from owners, cats’ purrs change, becoming more ‘urgent’ and ‘less
pleasant’ (McComb et al. 2009). When asking for food, a high-frequency
miaow is usually also embedded within the lower-pitch purr. However,
whether this food solicitation call is specific to cats’ relationship to
humans or whether they use it in other contexts, is currently unknown. Kittens have around 9 different types of vocalisationAttachment to owner
In 2007, Edwards et al. carried out the unusually-named ‘Ainsworth
Strange Situation Test’ in order to test whether cats were more attached
to their owners than to a random human. In this test, the cat was
essentially placed in a room and experienced being alone, being with
their human owner and being with an unknown human. The researchers found
that cats spent more time allogrooming (head-butting) their owners than
the stranger. They also only ever followed and played with their owner
and never with the stranger. The cats were generally more exploratory
and moved around more when their owner was in the room compared to the
stranger. Both when alone and with the stranger, the cat generally spent
more time being alert and sitting by the door. They vocalised the most
when alone (compared to when with either human). Thus it seems that cats
do have attachment to their owners that is stronger than with a random
human, which is perhaps somewhat comforting to know.
Cats also seem to experience separation anxiety, which also indicates
that they feel attachment to their owners. When separated from their
human owners, cats are more likely to display stress behaviours such as
urinating and defecating in inappropriate locations, excessive
vocalisation, destructiveness and excessive grooming.
While the studies that exist on cat cognition have helped illuminate
some of the abilities of our elusive housemates, there are still large
parts of cat behaviour that remain understudied and mean we still don’t
understand many aspects of cat behaviour. A greater understanding of
cats’ behaviour and our influence on it will lead to better human-cat
interactions, cat welfare and therefore the number of cats that are
given to shelters and euthanized.
Main reference
Shreve, K. R. V., & Udell, M. A. (2015). What’s inside your cat’s head? A review of cat (Felis silvestris catus) cognition research past, present and future. Animal cognition, 18, 1195-1206. Other references
Edwards, C., Heiblum, M., Tejeda, A., & Galindo, F. (2007). Experimental evaluation of attachment behaviors in owned cats. Journal of Veterinary Behavior:Clinical Applications and Research, 2, 119-125.
McComb K, Taylor AM, Wilson C, Charlton BD (2009) The cry embedded within the purr. Current Biology 19, R507–R508.
Miklósi, Á., Pongrácz, P., Lakatos, G., Topál, J., & Csányi, V.
(2005). A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals
in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 179.
Saito, A., & Shinozuka, K. (2013). Vocal recognition of owners by domestic cats (Felis catus). Animal cognition, 16, 685-690.
No comments:
Post a Comment